It is improbable that more nonsense has been written about aesthetics than about anything else: the literature of the subject is not large enough for that (Clive Bell)

Index

*********************
*********************
The Index is found here
*********************
*********************

Saturday, August 31, 2013

Kandel: Top Down Processing of Information

Kandel : Top Down Processing of Information: Using Memory to Find Meaning
This is Chapter 18 of Eric Kandel's "The Age of Insight : The Quest to Understand the Unconscious in Art, Mind, and Brain".

Quoted text is in YELLOW. Text quoted from other authors is in GREEN *************************************************************************** ****************************************************************************

Klimt, Field of Poppies, 1907



WHEN A GREAT ARTIST CREATES AN IMAGE OUT OF HIS OR HER OWN experience, that image is inherently ambiguous. As a result, the meaning of the image depends upon each viewer’s associations, know1edge of the world and of art, and ability to recall that knowledge and bring to bear on the particular image. This is the basis of the beholder's share—the re-creation of a work of art by the viewer. Cultural symbols, recalled from memory, are similarly critical for the producing and viewing of art. This has led Ernst Gombrich to argue that memory plays a critical role in the perception of art. In fact, as Gombrich emphasized, every painting owes more to other paintings than it does to its own internal content.





 That might indeed serve as an article of faith among art historians. But on the  contrary, one might assert that internal content is what makes a painting part of art history in the first place - even if  that assertion contradicts  the Institutional theory of art.


One might add that, barring physical deterioration, internal content never changes - while memory is transitory.




If, for example, we look at a landscape painted by Gustav Klimt, such as A Field of Poppies  it is difficult to ascertain the meaning of the image from the internal content alone. What is immediately apparent is a homogenous expanse of green paint, punctuated  with spots of red, blue, yellow, and white, stabilized by two small passages of white at the top edge of the canvas. Once we compare this image to what we know about painting, however, the content of the  picture becomes perfectly clear. Considered in the tradition of landscape painting, specifically that of Impressionist and Post-Impressionist  pointillists, what emerges from the mass of green and red splotches is a beautiful pastoral scene of a poppy field covered in flowers. Even  though the two trees in the foreground are difficult  to distinguish from the field of flowers they are growing in.
The beholder is easily able to construct a figure ground relationship between the two because he or she knows to look for them


Doesn't the search for the meaning of a painting -- or indeed anything  --- begin with determining what one is looking for ?

If  we had sent Klimt  to explore a remote island and this painting was included in his report -- yes, it would be very important for us to identify the trees, flowers, topography, etc, that he was depicting.

Or -- if we were trying to connect Klimt to the history of art -- then it would be important to know about Impressionism, Post-Impressionism, Pointilism etc.

But if we're just looking for  what might be  thrilling, satisfying, and distinctive about one  artist's unique visual world --- none of the above is necessarily relevant.










Klimt's  Poppy Field feels like an overwhelming, all-encompassing sensual experience -- as one might feel in a room dominated by  the above 16th C. Persian carpet.









As this brief essay explains, there are many and  various motifs to be found in this genre.

But does enumerating them even begin to describe the mood that the piece has created?



 Or -- what about enumerating the flora and fauna in the 'Narcissus' tapestry from Boston ?






The greater realism in  Klimt's poppy field  needs no scholarly footnotes to explain.

Unlike the two tapestries shown above,  there's an horizon line -- and the spots get smaller and more tightly compacted as one moves deeper into the imagined pictorial box -- just as they would if you were standing in an actual poppy field.

Is sensing this depth of pictorial space a "top-down" or "bottom-up" mental activity ?

 

Information processed from the bottom up relies in good part on the built-in architecture of the early stages of the visual system, which is largely the same for all viewers of a work of art. In contrast, down processing relies on mechanisms that assign categories and meaning and on prior knowledge, which is stored as memory in other regions of the brain. As a result, top-down processing is unique in each viewer



But isn't bottom up mental activity unique to each individual as well ?  Even if categories and meaning is not being assigned,  aren't  visual patterns and rhythms felt differently among various individuals ?

 









 Here are four versions of another  painting of a poppy field - the original done in 1873  by  Claude Monet.

The above is the reproduction provided online by the  Musee D'Orsay.










Here's a version that seems to have been through photoshop to juice up the colors and contrasts.








Here's a re-painted version that makes the figures and the house more distinct.








And here's another re-painted version for people who really like big red poppies.


Top-down : all that distinguishes them is the provenance of the image.  All four have identical categories of objects (trees, poppies, house, umbrella, child, etc)  on display in relatively identical  positions.


But bottom-up, some of these images are far more preferable to some people than to others.

I suspect that the one from the D'Orsay is closest to how the painting now appears -- though the bright  redness of the poppies may have dramatically faded over the past 140 years.

For me, the photoshop version is badly mangled -- while the two  re-paintings are monstrosities.

But that's just my lame brain going from bottom to top --- obviously many other brains feel quite differently.






THE IMPORTANCE OF top-down processing and the effects of attenion and memory on perception are immediately evident in viewing a work of art. To begin with, the attention of a person viewing a work of .art differs in important ways from what the student of perception Pascal Mamassian of the University of Paris calls “everyday perception.” In everyday perception the task is specified. If you are going to cross ii ic street, you watch for a break in the traffic before starting. Your perception is therefore highly focused on the cars coming by, their speed and size, and you disregard irrelevant information such as whether a particular car is a Buick or a Mercedes-Benz, whether it is gray or blue. It is much more difficult to identify an appropriate task in the perception of visual arts. Indeed, the viewer approaches a work of art differently, and his or her response can depend on where he or she is standing. The artist, in turn, faces the challenge of not knowing where the beholder will stand -and viewing angle can greatly affect the beholder's interpretation of a three-dimensional scene.



Yes -- just what is an "appropriate task in the viewing of a work of art"?

I'm glad Kandel told us that it is "difficult to identify"

But his first response is to suggest  that the viewer's response can depend on where the viewer is standing - and quotes Pascal Mamassian who asserts that "the only viewpoint from where the painting is a faithful rendition of the scene is the center of projection, that is the location where the painter was standing if the scene was painted on a transparent canvas"


I find this emphasis on the viewer's position puzzling, to say the least -- mostly because I doubt both the clarity and importance of whatever Kandel and Mamassin might mean by the phrase "faithful rendition"


Gustav Klimt, 1917


The only example that Kandel offers is the above drawing by Klimt

Understanding the nature of this top-down phenomenon as it pertains to where the beholder is standing teaches us a great deal about how we perceive paintings. The best-known example is when the eyes of the subject of a portrait follow us wherever we stand. This effect which we see in Gustav Klimt’s beautiful portrait  depends on the sitter’s having been painted looking at the painter’s eyes: The pupils of the sitter’s eyes will then be centered on our own eyes. When we move to the side, the position of the eye becomes distorted, but top-down perceptual system corrects for this distortion. As a result the pupil of the sitter’s eye is not altered very much, even though other distortions occur in the painting. This accounts for the illusion that the eyes of the subject follow us as we move. If the perspective in the painting were perfect, as it is in a sculpture, the portrait would look distorted from every perspective except one. Thus, if we view a bust from the side, the pupil of the eye appears asymmetrical and does not appear to be looking at us.


Wouldn't it be sufficient to say that a piece should be viewed from wherever it looks the best?

Kandel  is following Manmassian who is following Gombrich who emphasizes pictorial realism in the history of Western European art.  It's an approach that appeals to a scientific mind - but ignores how often realistic criteria - especially the single P.O.V. so important to Mamassian - have been ignored  before, during, and after the Renaissance.


But then, curiously enough, Kandel argues the antithesis : "the appreciation of art, and even our understanding of what it represents, does  not depend on our assuming any specific viewing position."

Then he quotes the cognitive psychologist, Patrick Cavanagh :The tolerance of flat representations is found in all cultures, infants, and in in other species, so it cannot result from learning a convention of representation."



Then he refers to Michael Polanyi ("Meaning", 1975) who distinguished focal awareness (of "the person or scene depicted")  from subsidiary awareness (of brushstrokes, marks etc) when viewing a painting.






This Van Gogh self portrait is offered as an example of those two states of awareness.

But can the mind distinguish  "the person or scene depicted" from the character and arrangement of the marks from which that depiction is recognized ?

Perhaps so -- if that mind is trying to identify the depicted details to answer questions like "who is this man?" or "what is he wearing?"

But what if the mind is asking "how do I feel about this image?"  - as my mind would ask when looking at works of art.   In that inquiry, the awareness of brushstrokes is not subsidiary to anything.

Some day, I suppose, I'll have to locate a copy of Polanyi's "Meaning" to discover how he got into his discussion of the viewing of paintings.  Perhaps it's  how he personally looks at art works.




Caravaggio. "Supper at Emmaus"




Regarding Kandel's discussion of viewing position --  I think it is important to assume the specific viewing position suggested by the painting. But it does not have to be one specific point.   In the "Supper at Emmaus", the fractured perspective is pulling the viewer across  the table, right up to the chest of Christ. (that's why the near and distant hands of the man on the right are the same size)


Although the aesthetic experience of a work of art is greater than the sum of its parts, the visual experience begins in this mosaic fashion, with a scanning of all the parts, one feature at a time..  The importance of different types of scanning movements in picking out the essential elements in a painting is illustrated in the following examples:





Klimt: Adele Bloch-Bauer II






Klimt: Adele Bloch-Bauer I






Klimt: Judith


Klimt’s three images of women—two of Adele Bloch-Bauer and one of Judith—differ primarily in their facial expression. In the top image, Adele is neutral, almost bored; in the second, she is modestly seductive; in the third, Judith, in the afterglow of sexual rapture, appears both victorious and ready for more encounters. But what is perhaps equally striking is how much of our visual attention, particularly for Adele Block-Bauer I and Adele Block-Bauer II, is expended on details having nothing to do with the sitter’s face, such as her richly ornamented gowns, which meld almost imperceptibly into the background. .... Thus in  viewing Klimt's portraits, our eyes are multitasking: they scan the image as a whole to obtain a sense of the various ideas the artist is  trying to convey.





Here's Klimt's portrait of Adele -- with everything but the face taken out.

To demonstrate -- I think --- that Klimt's background is giving Adele personality and character, as well as establishing her opulent queen-like social status.

And, at least to my sensibilities, she's only "modestly seductive" when that intense, formidable background has been removed -- but then, perhaps that's just because my own middle class background is intimidated by all that bling.

Regarding "multitasking" --- isn't that how one looks at something as art -- feeling whatever can be felt and relating to whatever memories are accessed at that moment?



















 In contrast, Van Gogh and Kokoschka invite the viewer to focus primarily on facial expression.  Each visage is unique and memorable. The richly textured background serves only to highlight the emotion that emerges from the facial features. Thus, in viewing Klimt's portraits, our eyes are multitasking: they scan the image as a whole to obtain a sense of the various ideas the artist is trying to convey.  In Kokoschka's portraits, our attention is tightly focused on the essential facial expression of each subject and the meaning of that expression.





And here is Kokoshka's portrait of Blumner with everything gone but the face.

The background did seem to  "highlight the  emotion that emerges from the facial features"

Though you could also say that the face highlights the emotion emerging from the background, so I would not agree with Kandel's assertion that   "in Kokoschka's portraits, our attention is tightly focused on the essential facial expression of each subject and the meahing of that expression"

And I don't think you could say the same for those Klimt portraits -- where the naturalistic face almost feels like it's been cut and pasted into a different kind of painting.










A very different type of contrast emerges from Kokoschka’s and Schiele’s depictions of themselves with a lover.  Kokoschka painted several romantic dual portraits of himself with Alma Mahler, but none of them speaks of consummation. In all of them Kokoschka appears passive, swept along by forces beyond his control. That impression is enhanced in The Wind’s Fiancée by the turbulent waves that surround the two lovers and that are beyond Kokoschka’s ability to control. Schiele, in contrast, specializes in raw sex: no lack of consummation here. But it is a sexual union in which the bliss of romance and erotic pleasure is counterbalanced or even overridden by anxiety. The mixture of conflicting instinctual drives cancels out any pleasurable component of sex, resulting in an emptiness—the emptiness of union—which can be seen in the eyes of the subjects. In Schiele’s portraits, the anxiety connected with sex is so great, and the sheer power of the emotion so strong, that he appears to need no tempest, no background effects, to dramatize it further.
The artists’ stylistic differences—differences that our eyes take in all at once—convey different emotional content. 





Seitei School, 1900-1910



By way of even greater contrast, here's a mating couple from the same decade, though from a very different tradition.

The personality of the lovers is absent.  Other than for the animation of their coupling,  they are plant-like. The turbulence is completely biological.  Their sex is a beautiful moment, rather than an emotional crisis of self identity - and it's more of a dream-like fantasy than a record of personal experience.



But why did Kandel compare those two paintings anyway ? His only conclusion is that "The artists’ stylistic differences—differences that our eyes take in all at once—convey different emotional content""
--- which could be said about any two images.

His discussion of the top-down or bottom-up  perception of visual art has been rather thin, and does nothing to establish that the art viewing mind doesn't continuously  bounce back and forth between top, bottom, and all points in between.


Sunday, August 18, 2013

Kandel : The Fusion of Eroticism, Aggression, and Anxiety in Art

Kandel : The Fusion of Eroticism, Aggression, and Anxiety in Art
This is Chapter 10 of Eric Kandel's "The Age of Insight : The Quest to Understand the Unconscious in Art, Mind, and Brain".

Quoted text is in YELLOW. Text quoted from other authors is in GREEN *************************************************************************** ****************************************************************************





IN HIS PALPABLE EXISTENTiAL ANXIETY, EGON SCRHELE  is the Franz Kafka of modern painting. While Gustav Klimt and Oskar Kokoschka, spurred by their intellectual contemporaries, took an interest in the inner lives of their subjects, Schiele more than any other artist of his time took an interest in his own anxiety. He expresses this deep anxiety—as if his private world were coming apart—in numerous self-portraits, and he superimposes a corresponding anxiety on everyone he painted, including the people in the dual portraits of his sexual experiences. There is, in those portraits, a frightening aloneness, even in union.








I've got to agree with Kandel's connection of Schiele to Kafka --- Schiele sees himself as a kind of insect --
and insects stay alive by living in perpetual anxiety.











And they  can  also be beautiful -- in a terrible  kind of way

Reminds me of a punk-ish young woman who wore black lipstick and modeled for us a few years ago.

On her forearms she had self-tattooed the crudely lettered words: KILL -- FUCK -- EAT --- DIE

. Like Klimt and Kokoschka, he was preoccupied with aggression and death, but his work, unlike Klimt’s drawings of women enjoying their sexuality, conveys a wider range of female emotion in response to sex—torment, guilt, anxiety, sadness, rejection, curiosity, and even surprise. Especially in his early work, Schiele’s women appear to suffer their sensuality rather than enjoy it.




1912





This was the only example I could find of an early work where a woman appeared to suffer rather than enjoy her sensuality.





1910


If   not just a figure study, his drawings often appear designed to satisfy a male taste for cheesecake --  like a pin-up girl.







In this sense, Schiele followed Kokoschka’s lead in attempting to probe deeply his own life and the lives of those he painted. But he differed from Kokoschka in several important regards. Rather than focusing exclusively on facial expressions and hand gestures to explore beneath the surface of his subjects and obtain insights into their character and conflicts, Schicle used the whole body. Also unlike Kokoschka, who frequently  painted other people,  Schicle often painted himself. He depicted himself as sad, anxious, deeply frightened, and sexually engaged with himself or with others.







Portrait of Gerti Schiele, 1909


As you can see from the above, Schiele came under the influence of Klimt who recognized his talent and encouraged him.




 Portrait of Anton Peschka, 1909




 Portrait of Anton Peschka, 1911










But as you see from these two portraits of his brother-in-law, he quickly moved on to a less decorative style




1910




A poor student in school, Schiele possessed an exceptional ability to draw. Based on that talent, he was admitted to the Vienna Academy of Ii iie Arts at age sixteen, where he was the youngest student in his class. He began to perfect his already formidable drawing skills by first simulating and then elaborating upon the technique of blind contour drawing that had been developed recently by the sculptor Auguste Rodin’ and adopted by Klimt. Schiele would observe his models and, without hiking his eyes off them or lifting his pencil off the paper, would draw their figures with extraordinary speed in one continuous line, which he never modified or erased.

Tlie result was a highly distinctive line that was at once nervous and precise—and very different from Klimt’s sensuous, Art Nouveau line or the meticulous and calculated rendering of the Viennese academic tradition. Using this new line, Schiele was able to capture the gestures and movements of his models and himself and to express them by means of contours rather than light and shadow. Schiele would continue to apply this drawing technique throughout his career, communicating evocative body language through the power of outline and silhouette.



Above is an example of an actual  blind contour drawing - as shown on the internet in observance of an exercise in Nicolaides' "Natural Way to Draw", a popular instructional book first published in 1941.



Like Schiele's drawing shown above it, there is no sense of volume or space.  But unlike Schiele's, there's also no sense of graphic design.

If the artist is not also looking at the paper, his lines cannot relate to it.

Schiele (and Rodin) definitely placed great emphasis on contour lines - and may well have drawn them without lifting the pencil off the paper. But his designs are too strong to have been done without looking at them.


Picasso, Portrait of Stravinsky, 1920


Same thing with the above drawing by Picasso.

Where did Kandel pick up the notion that Schiele made drawings without looking them? He doesn't provide any footnotes for it, and I can't find anything on Google beyond a brief mention on "How to Draw" websites.




The result was a highly distinctive line that was at once nervous and precise - and very different from Klimt's sensuous, Art Nouveau line   or the meticulous and calculated rendering of the Viennese academic tradition.


There's no disputing that - and it does seem that the early 20th C. saw a brief but wonderful emergence of depiction that was both based on direct  observation and  strongly contoured.  I can't think of another period of European, or even world, art history that compares with it.  It disappeared as figurative depiction became either more decorative (Art Deco) or more angst ridden (Surrealism)










 In his search for what lies below the surface of everyday life, Schiele, like Kokoschka, was a true contemporary of Freud and Schnitzler: he studied the psyche and believed implicitly that to understand another person’s unconscious processes, he had first to understand his own. Schiele exhibited himself compulsively in his drawings and paintings— All of Schiele’s self-portraits depict him in front of a mirror, sometimes in the act of masturbating 

The paintings of himself masturbating are bold on several levels, not the least of which is that many people in Vienna at that time thought duit masturbation by men led to insanity.

But the self-portraits are not simply an exhibition of nudity; they are an attempt at full disclosure of the self, a self-analysis, a pictorial version of Freud’s The Interpretation of Dreams. In an essay entitled  “Live Flesh,” the philosopher and art critic Arthur Danto has written:



 Eroticism and pictorial representation have co-existed since the beginning of art. .. . But Schiele was unique in making eroticism the defining motif of his impressive . . . oeuvre. [Schiele’s paintings] are like illustrations of a thesis of Sigmund Freud . . . that human reality is essentially sexual. What I mean is there is no art historical explanation  of Schiele's vision.





An image search for "self  portrait masturbating" only pulled up two examples -- and only one artist: Schiele.

So even back in Freud's Vienna, he may have been the only artist to depict that theme.

He doesn't look especially happy  about it does he ?



self portrait, 1910






self portrait, 1910



Kandel asserts that both of the above self portraits also depict masturbation , but if so, then the same would be true for every solitary nude.



Richard Gerstl (1883-1908), self portrait






An image search for "nude self portrait" also brings very few results prior to contemporary sexting and post-modern photography.

But interestingly enough, it did find another Viennese contemporary of Schiele, Richard Gerstl, who regretfully killed himself after getting dumped by the wife of Arnold Schoenberg.




Gertstl: Portrait of the Schoenberg Family, 1908


Gerstl was quite a painter - it's too bad he lost control of his personal life.



 Albrecht Durer, 1505


And here's a more famous nude self-portrait - this one done 500 years before Sigmund Freud.











And how would Kandel compare this to the self portraits by Schiele?

It does seem to have an expressive character - as well as angst.

Eroticism - aggression- anxiety ?    Maybe.

But he definitely does not present himself as a clown or misfit or nut case.

(BTW -  this drawing is currently in the Schloss Museum, Linz -- about 150 miles from Vienna. Was Schiele familiar with it ? )







Lucian Freud, "Painter working, reflection", 1993



And then, more recently, we have Sigmund Freud's grandson, in an image that may be nude but doesn't seem to have anything to do with sexuality.

And though you feel his soul has been laid bare -- he doesn't seem to be crazy at all -- just devoted to his  calling.












In his 1915 Self_Portrait with Striped Armlets, Schiele presents himself as a social misfit, a clown or fool . The armlets, with their vertical stripes, recall the typical costume of a court jester. The artist has colored his hair bright orange, and his wide_open eyes hint at madness. His head tilts precariously from the top of a slender neck.

Similar to his contemporary, Picasso, who identified with Harlequin.











 In another self-portrait, the force of his anxious expression is further heightened by the application of a thick white halo of gouache around the outlines of his head, isolating it and making it stand out against the background while at the same time making it appear large and deserving of emphasis. Moreover, Schiele depicts an immense territory above his eyes—a gigantic forehead and a deeply furrowed brow. This portrait suggests that Schiele may have wanted to recapitulate Klimt’s earlier depiction of the decapitated Holofernes, but with himself as the victim: he has placed his head at the top of the sheet of paper, emphasizing that his body is missing.



Rudolph Weisenborn (1881-1974)

Here's a self portrait by  a Chicago artist from about the same time. He looks pretty angry and antisocial,  but by comparison, Schiele looks like he belongs in a psychiatric hospital.


Quoting art  historian, Allesadra Comini "Why did Schiele's work assume such imperious intensity at this time and how did he fashion a new artistic vocabulary that called for this concentrated vision"?

Was it because he had seen his father go mad from a syphilitic brain disorder ? Or because the cultural elite of Vienna were then so  focused on the psyche ? A physician friend invited him to draw the patients in his mental clinic.

There seemed to be a greater curiosity regarding madness than sanity, possibly  because physicians and scientists were so  dominant among the cultural elite.  So Man was more like a biological specimen than an active participant in either public or sacred narratives.



1918



Some of Schiele’s images, unlike those of the earlier artists, focus explicitly on genitalia and sexual acts. In Crouching Female Nude with Bent Head of 1918, for example, Schiele conveys a girl’s feelings by depicting her with her head deeply bowed and an expression of wistful melancholy on her face. Long, loose strands of hair frame her face, as if she were searching for protection and security.









Making Love, 1915




In paintings such as Love Making of 1915, sexuality, eroticism, world-weariness, exhuaustion, and fear fuse to express the inseparability of Eros and anxiety.



I don't feel any Eros in the embrace of these two puppets. Isn't this what you'd call "cuddling"?






Death and the Maiden, 1815

As Kandel explains it, this painting depicts the artist and his mistress, Wally, at that delicate  moment after they have made love  and he has decided to dump her  Self centered as the artist may have been, this character does seem to express remorse regarding his soon-to-be-abandoned lover - as the artist seeks a more socially acceptable match.

So more than just psychological, this painting is also interpersonal -- and maybe even moral - as the artist sees his lover as vulnerable and himself as a wretch.

I wonder what happened to Wally?

She probably outlived Egon who died three years later.

But unlike Picasso's early mistresses, she vanished from the public eye.








Death and the Maiden is often compared to Kokoschka’s The Wind’s Iiancée, depicting his tumultuous relationship with Alma Mahier, but the two paintings are actually quite different. In both works, the men are anxious, but in The Wind Fiancée Alma Mahler is sleeping calmly, whereas in Death and the Maiden Wally is experiencing a sense of isolation and desperation comparable to Schiele’s own. She senses abandonment, he a lack of fulfillment. In Schiele’s world no one is ever safe.






It's too bad these two paintings cannot be permanently displayed side by side. Have any earlier paintings in art history depicted the emotional disasters of romantic love as frankly as these two ? The only thing worse than a lover who clings -- is a lover who doesn't.













 The doppelganger, a popular theme of German Romantic literature, is the ghostly counterpart of a person who acts just as the person does. Although the doppe1ganger can take the form of a protector or imaginary companion, it is often a harbinger of death. In folklore, the doppe1gnger is a phantom of the self: it casts no shadow and has no reflection in a mirror. Schiele uses the doppelgänger in both senses in his double self-portraits. In Death and Man (Self-Seers II) of 1911, Schiele fuses what appears to be either his own face or his father’s face with the skeleton-like figure of death standing behind him. As with many of Schiele’s works, the image is both frightening and intriguing.


If, as Kandel asserts, this image relates to a physic phenomenon of German folk-lore, it hardly seems connected to Freud and modern medical science.

I can relate to  it personally as a depiction of father and son - especially since that ghostly figure in the background resembles my own father in his last decade.




Hermits, 1912


“In the large painting one doesn’t see exactly how the two are standing there, at first glance ---this is important to the painting --- otherwise, the poetic idea and the vision would be lost, as would the ambiguity of the figures which, conceived as being crumpled into themselves, are the bodies of individuals who have grown tired of life, grown suicidal—but even so, they are people of emotion. —Think of these two as being like a cloud of dust similar to this Earth, a cloud which wants to grow into something more but must necessarily collapse, its strength spent.

                          ........... Egon Schiele in a letter to an important German patron/collector of modern art





As Kandel notes, this double portrait of Schiele with Klimt echoes the one done by the youthful Kokoschka, where the young artist is leaning on the shoulder of the older one.






 But in Schiele’s portrait, Klimt—Schiele’s irIistic father—is leaning on him for support. In 1912 Klimt was still at the peak of his career, the dominant force in the artistic world of Vienna, yet in the painting he seems barely to hang on, not only to Schiele to life itself. In fact, his wide, blank eyes suggest that he is blind. Schiele’s double portrait may well reflect the artist’s unconscious, Oedipal  desire to eliminate his imagined rival, Klimt, and succeed him as Veiiiia’s top artist.





This is a small community, isn't it? With Klimt as the village headman. Did any other modern artist of that period take as much interest in mentoring the next generation of artists ? (and one might note -- Klimt was not teaching in an art school)

Would Kokoschka and Schiele have enjoyed the same early success without Klimt's involvement?







 Cardinal and Nun (The Caress)  1912








Kandel shows us more evidence of Schiele's connection to Klimt, this parody of Klimt's  "The Kiss", with Wally posing as the nun.








The guilt-ridden darkness of Schiele's version certainly brings out the romantic idealism in Klimt's.




Schiele's death  marked the end of the  expressionist era in Vienna, an era that saw the first step toward a potential dialogue between science and art.  The five giants who emerged from Vienna 1900 could trace their immense accomplishments in psychoanalysis,  literature, and art directly or indirectly to the  the scientific influence of Rokitansky's view that surface appearances are deceptive and that to obtain the truth, we need to go  deep below the surface


And so, with the premature death of Schiele from influenza in 1918,  Kandel ends this chapter




Here are  the paintings I could find from the last two years -- and they don't seem to be about an "inner truth" any more than the figure paintings of Manet, Eakins, Goya, and Rembrandt - to name just a few.



1918



When Schiele was living the Bohemian life -- that's what he painted.



1917 






But now that he's becoming a respectable middle class family man - that seems to be where his vision is going.


1917



He still likes a little kinkyness - but nothing that would shock a banker.




1917




And he's even moving into cute.





1917